雅思阅读: Teaching you children
When should you teach children, and when should you let them explore?
ITis one of the oldest debates in education. Should teachers tell pupils the way things are or encourage them to find out for themselves? Telling children truths about the world helps them learn those facts more quickly. Yet the efficient learning of specific facts may lead to the assumption that when the adult has finished teaching, there is nothing further to learnbecause if there were, the adult would have said so. A study just published in Cognition by Elizabeth Bonawitz of the University of California, Berkeley, and Patrick Shafto of the University of Louisville, in Kentucky, suggests that is true.
Dr Bonawitz and Dr Shafto arranged for 85 four- and five-year-olds to be presented, during a visit to a museum, with a novel toy that looked like a tangle of coloured pipes and was capable of doing many different things. They wanted to know whether the way the children played with the toy depended on how they were instructed by the adult who gave it to them.
One group of children had a strictly pedagogical introduction. The experimenter said Look at my toy! This is my toy. Im going to show you how my toy works. She then pulled a yellow tube out of a purple tube, creating a squeaking sound. Following this, she said, Wow, see that? This is how my toy works! and then demonstrated the effect again.
With a second group of children, the experimenter acted differently. She interrupted herself after demonstrating the squeak by saying she had to go and write something down, thus suggesting that she might not have finished the demonstration. With a third group, she activated the squeak as if by accident. To a fourth, the toy was simply presented with the comment, Wow, see this toy? Look at this!
After these varied introductions, the children were left with the toy and allowed to play. They might discover that, as well as the squeaker, the toy had a button inside one tube which activated a light, a keypad that played musical notes, and an inverting mirror inside one of the tubes. All the children were told to let the experimenter know when they had finished playing and were asked by the instructor if they were done if they stopped playing for more than five consecutive seconds. The entire interaction was recorded on video.
Footage of each child playing was passed to a research assistant who was ignorant of the purpose of the study. The assistant was asked to record the total playing time, the number of different actions the child performed, the time spent playing with the squeak, and the number of other functions the child discovered.
The upshot was that children in the first group spent less time playing than those in the second , the third or the fourth . Those in the first group also tried out four different actions, on average. The others tried 5.3, 5.9 and 6.2, respectively. A similar pattern pertained to the number of functions other than the squeak that the children found.
The researchers conclusion was that, in the context of strange toys of unknown function, prior explanation does, indeed, inhibit exploration and discovery. Generalising from that would be ambitious. But it suggests that further research might be quite a good idea.
公主殿下的单词你会讲吗
初中英语教学随笔
初学者如何练习英语口语
高一英语期末教学总结
谈对英语学困生的兴趣培养
好的教师关键在于好的教学方法
高职高专英语教学随笔
初中英语小班化教学探讨
初一上学期英语教学反思
“真名天子”英文如何说
在餐馆点菜常说的13句话
开斋节的英语单词
谈培养学生英语学习的良好习惯
郁金香的英语单词如何讲
如何增强英语复习课的活力
茄子的英语单词如何讲
浅谈英语歌曲在英语教学中的作用
五种趣味方法帮你学好初中英语语法
用美国人思维学习口语的六大技巧
初一英语教学反思
爵士用英文怎么表达
袋鼠英语单词如何讲
海浪的英语单词如何讲
小学英语教学随笔
谈小学英语教师的人文素养
薰衣草英语单词
天使除了“angel”还能怎么讲
英语教学课堂导入的方法
中学英语教师随笔
纠结的英语单词
不限 |
英语教案 |
英语课件 |
英语试题 |
不限 |
不限 |
上册 |
下册 |
不限 |