Who s the Smart Sibling?
Ten weeks ago, Bo Cleveland and his wife embarked on a highly unscientific experiment-they gave birth to their first child. For now, Cleveland is too exhausted to even consider having another baby, but eventually, he will. In fact, hes already planned an egalitarian strategy for raising the rest of his family. Little Arthur won t get any extra attention just because he s the firstborn, and, says his father, he probably won t be much smarter than his future .siblings; either.It s the sort of thing many parents would say, but it s a bit surprising coming from Cleveland,who studies birth order and IQ at Pennsylvania State University. As he knows too well, a study published recently in the journal Science suggests that firstborns do turn out sharper than their brothers and sisters, no matter how parents try to compensate. Is Cleveland wrong? Is Arthur destined to be the smart sibling just because he had the good luck to be born first?
For decades, scientists have been squabbling over birth order like siblings fighting over a toy. Some of them say being a first-, middle- or lastborn has significant effects on intelligence. Others say that s nonsense, The spat goes back at least as far as Alfred Adler, a Freud-era psychologist who argued that firstborns had an edge. Other psychologists found his theory easy to believemiddle and youngest kids already had a bad rap, thanks to everything from primogeniture laws to the Prodigal Son. When they set out to confirm the birth-order effects Adler had predicted, they found some evidence. Dozens of studies over the next several decades showed small differences in IQ; scholastic-aptitude tests and other measures of achievement So did anecdata suggesting that firstborns were more likely to win Nobel Prizes or become prominent psychologists.
But even though the scientists were turning up birth-order patterns easily, they couldn t pin down a cause. Perhaps, one theory went, the mother s body was somehow attacking the later offspring in uterus. Maternal antibody levels do increase with each successive pregnancy. But there s no evidence that this leads to differences in intelligence, and the new study in Silence,based on records from nearly a quarter of a million young Norwegian men, strikes down the antibody hypothesis. It looks at kids who are the eldest by accident-those whose older siblings die in infancy--as well as those who are true firstborns. Both groups rack up the same high scores on IQ tests. Whatever is lowering the latterborns scores, it isn t prenatal biology, since being raised as the firstborn, not actually being the firstborn, is what counts.
The obvious culprits on the nurture side are parents. But it s hard to think that favoritism toward firstborns exists in modem society. Most of us no longer view secondborn as second best, and few parents will admit to treating their kids differently. In surveys, they generally say they give their children equal attention. Kids concur, reporting that they feel they re treated fairly.
Maybe, then, the problem with latterborns isn t nature or nurture-maybe there simply isn t a problem. Not all the research shows a difference in intelligence. A pivotal 2000 study by Joe Rodgers ,now a professor emeritus at the University of Oklahoma, found no link between birth order and smarts. And an earlier study of American families found that the youngest kids, not the oldest, did best in school. From that work, say psychologist Judith Rich Harris, a prominent critic of birth-order patterns, it s clear that the impression that the firstborn is more often the academic achiever is false.
Meanwhile, many of the studies showing a birth-order pattern in IQ have a big, fat,methodological flaw. The Norwegian Science study is an example, says Cleveland: It s comparing Bill, the first child in one family; to Bob, the second child in another family. That would be fine if all families were identical, but of course they aren t. The study controls for variables such as parental education and family size. But Rodgers, the Oklahoma professor,notes that there are hundreds of other factors in play; and because it s so hard to discount all of them, he s not sure whether the patterns in the Science article are real.
No one is more sensitive to that criticism than the Norwegian scientists. In fact, they already have an answer ready in the form of a second paper. Soon to be published in the journal Intelligence, it s, similar to the Science study except for one big thing: instead of comparing Bill to Bob, it compares Bill to younger brothers Barry and Barney. The same birth- order pattern shows up: the firstborns, on average, score about two points higher than their secondborn brothers, and hapless thirdborns do even worse. The purpose of the two papers was exactly the same, says Petter Kristensen of Norway s National Institute of Occupational Health, who led both new studies. But this second one is much more comprehensive, and in a sense it s better than the Science paper. The data are there--within families, birth order really does seem linked to brain power. Even the critics have to soften their positions a little. The Intelligence study must be taken very seriously says Rodgers.
No one, not even Kristensen, thinks the debate is over For one thing, there s still that argument about what s causing birth-order effects. It s possible, says UC Berkeley researcher Frank Sulloway, that trying .to treat kids in an evenhanded way in fact results in inequity. Well-meaning parents may end up shortchanging middleborns because there s one thing they can t equalize: at no point in the middle child s life does he get to be the only kid in the house. Alternatively, says Sulloway; there s the theory he has his money on, the family-niche hypothesis Older kids, whether out of desire or necessity axe often called on to be assistant parents, he notes. Getting that early- taste of responsibility may prime them for achievement later on. If they think Oh, I m supposed to be more intelligent so I d better do my homework, it doesn t matter if they actually are more-intelligent, says Sulloway, It becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy. If the firstborns homework involves reading Science and Intelligence, there ll be no stopping them now.
小升初英文奥数试题(四)
小升初英语作文范文:Snowman
小升初英语必考考点:动词do及have/has用法口诀用法口诀
牛津小学英语6A Unit1 Public signs单元提高试卷
小学英语练习题精选--(试卷资料)
小学六年级英语阅读竞赛初赛试题
小升初英文奥数试题(二)
小升初英语必考考点:小学英语形容词比较级顺口溜
牛津小学英语6A Unit1过关测试
2012年小升初英语试题及答案(下)
六年级英语复习专项题――句型变换
小升初英语奥数题
六年级英语综合练习(2)和答案
小升初英语必考考点:小学PEP英语句型(21-40)
2012年小升初英语试题及答案(上)
小学英语六年级综合练习(5)及答案
小升初英语必考考点:助动词用法口诀
六年级英语测试2006-3-27
小升初英语作文范文:my weekend
牛津版小学英语五年级(5A unit 6-10)单元检测试题复习(笔试部分)
牛津英语6B总复习试卷
小升初英语必考考点:小学PEP英语句型(1-20)
小升初英语作文范文:I have a best friend
小升初英语作文范文:自我介绍
小升初英语作文范文:National Day
07年小升初英语专项归纳复习练习(3)
小升初英语必考考点汇总
小升初英文奥数试题(三)
小升初英语作文范文:我自己
小升初英语必考考点:小学PEP英语句型(41-66)
不限 |
英语教案 |
英语课件 |
英语试题 |
不限 |
不限 |
上册 |
下册 |
不限 |