雅思阅读:Now you know
When should you teach children, and when should you let them explore?
IT IS one of the oldest debates in education. Should teachers tell pupils the way things are or encourage them to find out for themselves? Telling children truths about the world helps them learn those facts more quickly. Yet the efficient learning of specific facts may lead to the assumption that when the adult has finished teaching, there is nothing further to learnbecause if there were, the adult would have said so. A study just published in Cognition by Elizabeth Bonawitz of the University of California, Berkeley, and Patrick Shafto of the University of Louisville, in Kentucky, suggests that is true.
Dr Bonawitz and Dr Shafto arranged for 85 four- and five-year-olds to be presented, during a visit to a museum, with a novel toy that looked like a tangle of coloured pipes and was capable of doing many different things. They wanted to know whether the way the children played with the toy depended on how they were instructed by the adult who gave it to them.
One group of children had a strictly pedagogical introduction. The experimenter said Look at my toy! This is my toy. Im going to show you how my toy works. She then pulled a yellow tube out of a purple tube, creating a squeaking sound. Following this, she said, Wow, see that? This is how my toy works! and then demonstrated the effect again.
With a second group of children, the experimenter acted differently. She interrupted herself after demonstrating the squeak by saying she had to go and write something down, thus suggesting that she might not have finished the demonstration. With a third group, she activated the squeak as if by accident. To a fourth, the toy was simply presented with the comment, Wow, see this toy? Look at this!
After these varied introductions, the children were left with the toy and allowed to play. They might discover that, as well as the squeaker, the toy had a button inside one tube which activated a light, a keypad that played musical notes, and an inverting mirror inside one of the tubes. All the children were told to let the experimenter know when they had finished playing and were asked by the instructor if they were done if they stopped playing for more than five consecutive seconds. The entire interaction was recorded on video.
Footage of each child playing was passed to a research assistant who was ignorant of the purpose of the study. The assistant was asked to record the total playing time, the number of different actions the child performed, the time spent playing with the squeak, and the number of other functions the child discovered.
The upshot was that children in the first group spent less time playing than those in the second , the third or the fourth . Those in the first group also tried out four different actions, on average. The others tried 5.3, 5.9 and 6.2, respectively. A similar pattern pertained to the number of functions other than the squeak that the children found.
The researchers conclusion was that, in the context of strange toys of unknown function, prior explanation does, indeed, inhibit exploration and discovery. Generalising from that would be ambitious. But it suggests that further research might be quite a good idea.
2015年职称英语考试《理工类》重点高频词组
2015年职称英语考试必备:730个高频词汇251-300
2015年职称英语考试备考:利用inter来记单词
2015年职称英语考试备考:用vi来进行联想记忆
职称英语成绩有效期是多久?
2015年职称英语考试《理工类》A级词汇选项练习题
2015职称英语考试《理工类》A级词汇选项练习题(5)
2015年职称英语考试备考:利用词根
2015年职称英语考试《综合类》高频固定搭配短语(5)
2015年职称英语考试备考:利用ate来记忆
2015年职称英语考试:备考单词记忆方式汇总
2015职称英语考试常用语法详解第二讲之基本句型(1)
2015年职称英语考试必备:730个高频词汇201-250
2015年职称英语词汇备考:基数词和序数词
2015年职称英语《理工类》重难点词组:动词+名词形式
2015年职称英语《理工类》重难点词组:动词+介词形式
2015年职称英语考试备考:利用部分单词的含义
2015年职称英语考试必备:730个高频词汇301-350
2015年职称英语考试必备:730个高频词汇401-450
2015年职称英语词汇备考:形容词
2015年职称英语考试备考:单词拆分记忆法
2015职称英语考试常用语法详解第三讲之并列复合句
职称英语考试须知的词汇选项题解题技巧
2015年职称英语《理工类》重难点词组:部分名词用法辨析
2015年职称英语考试《理工类》重点语法词组
2015年职称英语考试理工类同义词
2015职称英语考试常用语法详解第二讲之基本句型(5)
2015年职称英语考试《理工类》高频同义词
2015年职称英语考试备考:Re和Port的用法
2015年职称英语考试必备:730个高频词汇101-150
不限 |
英语教案 |
英语课件 |
英语试题 |
不限 |
不限 |
上册 |
下册 |
不限 |